Table of Contents Table of Contents
Previous Page  161 /304 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 161 /304 Next Page
Page Background

161

臺大管理論叢

28

卷第

1

the effect of popularity information is consistent. Finally, the student population in this

research is relatively high (48%). This leads to the result that the age distribution in this

research is significantly lower compared to other research. However, responses were

voluntary, thus, inevitably subject to self-selection biases. An additional K-S test was

performed to examine the collected responses between the students and non-students, and

the result showed no significance. Furthermore, students and young people are known as a

major population for online purchases (Dai, Forsythe, and Kwon, 2014). Thus, it is

legitimate to assume that there may be more student respondents to our online survey than

of other age groups. Much research even aims to examine the student sample when

conducting Internet-related studies (Kuo and Wu, 2012; Lin and Lekhawipat, 2014; Yeh

and Li, 2014). This leads us to believe that our sample in this experiment is appropriate

and reflects the characteristics of the target Internet users. An additional K-S test was

performed to examine the collected responses between the students and non-students and

the result shows no significant difference. Nevertheless, we still do not rule out the

possibility that subjects’ ages may sometimes exert influence on consumers’ online

purchase behavior. Therefore, generalization of our research, especially to older people,

may have limitations. Future research conducted with different age groups is strongly

recommended.

5.6 Future Research

There are many interesting future directions that could be derived from this research.

For example, our research has mentioned that social comparison may be a possible drive

to exert the social inference. Different kinds of social comparison may be triggered when

consumers compare with someone similar (e.g., other consumers) or someone better (e.g.,

experts and celebrities) for different motivations. Future research could focus even more

on this area and explore social comparison with different directions (e.g., someone similar,

better-off, and worse-off) and different motivations (e.g., self-evaluation and self-

enhancement). Another interesting direction would be to examine consumers’ emotions

when different kinds of inferences are induced. Particularly for the social inference, past

literature has indicated that consumers perceive stronger happiness and disappointment

when social inference (particularly when the effect of social comparison) is induced

(Ackerman, MacInnis, and Folkes, 2000; Hoch and Loewenstein, 1991; Wu and Lee,

2008a, 2008b). Consistent with this prediction, the results of our manipulation check show

that participants with the social inference feel marginally more disappointed when the